Alsueno Bull Terriers and Training

Alsueno Bull Terriers and Training Hi im Nancy a certified pro dog trainer..

I love helping dogs and there owners with lots of skills from barking and lunging reactivity seperation related struggles calmness boundary training and loose lead walking a favourite with us all…

August Specials                    Reactivity 1-1 in home plan                    Free phone consult                    ...
06/08/2025

August Specials

Reactivity 1-1 in home plan
Free phone consult
Up to 3.5 hours
$100.00

Loose lead walking and
off leash freedom
+ recall
+emergency stop.
Free phone consult
$80 first lesson
$60 for check in.

Multi- dog households
Free phone consult
Disengagement
Boundaries
$100

Message for any behaviour struggles your dog is having and we can set up a plan..

Certified UK pro-dog trainer.

I have been anxiousI can be fearfulAnd reactiveI have had noise phobia and high prey drive…But with the right skills  CO...
05/08/2025

I have been anxious
I can be fearful
And reactive
I have had noise phobia and high prey drive…
But with the right skills CONFIDENCE, OPTIMISM, CALM AND GRIT…
I am skilled up to live my best life..

Concept skills for a reinforcement rich life..
Off lead feeedom
Focus
Confidence ❤️

Super interesting 😊
03/08/2025

Super interesting 😊

“Heads up”: What Reinforces a Behaviour That Predicts Something (Unavoidably) Unpleasant?

This morning, a dog came bolting across the dunes when they spotted Juno. She was nose-deep in driftwood, blissfully unaware.

“Heads up,” I called.

She lifted her head and scanned the horizon. Just like she always does after I give her the warning.

What’s interesting is that the behaviour — the head lift, the look around — is deliberate, consistent, and highly reliable. She does it every time I say it. But what follows isn’t always particularly pleasant in the way we might traditionally think of r+. A dog running straight toward her is rarely a welcome experience. Juno can tolerate a quick hello, but it’s clear she finds these greetings intrusive, especially when the other dog doesn’t slow down or offer a pause to “ask.” She stiffens slightly, shifts behind me, sometimes squints or turns her head. She rarely reacts loudly because she’s learned quieter strategies that are usually effective and because I do everything I can to intercept early.

Still, despite the event that follows often being seemingly mildly aversive, the cue and the behaviour are very much maintained. So, what gives?

Let’s break this down:
🔹 Antecedent: I say “heads up.”
🔹 Behaviour: Juno lifts her head and looks around to locate the dog.
🔹 Consequence: She gains information about what’s coming, and sometimes the ability to act on it.

So, if the arrival of the dog is the consequence, and it’s not reinforcing… what is?

The answer isn’t as simple as “treats” or “praise.”

What’s reinforcing may be the reduction in uncertainty.

Juno isn’t orienting because she enjoys being greeted by dogs that ignore social boundaries. She’s orienting because it gives her a sliver of control, a chance to prepare, and a way to communicate “no thanks” before the other dog gets too close.
And in behavioural terms, that matters.

What the Literature Tells Us:

📌 Predictability is emotionally protective
Even when the outcome isn’t great, knowing it’s coming can reduce distress. In both human and nonhuman animals, predictable aversive events are less emotionally damaging than unpredictable ones (Seligman & Maier, 1967; Mineka & Hendersen, 1985).

📌 Control (even partial) has intrinsic value
Perceived control has measurable stress-reducing effects (Leotti, Iyengar, & Ochsner, 2010). Juno can’t always stop the dog, but she can prepare, shift her body, or seek support from me. That sliver of control is reinforcing.

📌 Orienting may be negatively reinforced
By looking around, Juno reduces the intensity of the aversive event. She’s less startled, better able to respond, and more able to subtly avoid escalation. That reduction in stress functions as a form of negative reinforcement.

📌 Discriminative control is strong when reinforcement history is mixed
Even if the current consequence (the dog) is mildly aversive, orienting may be maintained by earlier reinforcement (praise, food, information, the occasional chance to disengage) (Ferster & Skinner, 1957; Nevin et al., 2001). Behaviour maintained under variable or intermittent reinforcement is surprisingly resilient.

📌 Habit formation and cue persistence
After many repetitions, a cue like “heads up” can become embedded in a behavioural routine. Even if the original reinforcers fade, the cue-response pattern persists, especially if it still yields some benefit (Balleine & Dickinson, 1998; Domjan, 2018).

There’s another layer here too. A social layer.

Looking around gives Juno the chance to act early. That might mean turning her head, shifting behind me, or making herself smaller. These subtle signals though often missed by most humans we meet, are meaningful in dog communication. And often, they work.

So even when she can’t fully avoid the interaction, she can change the terms of it. And that’s reinforcing too. This fits with what we know about early signal detection and the importance of proactive behaviour in social encounters (Wemelsfelder, 2007; Wiley, 2006).

🧠 Reinforcement Isn’t Always Obvious
What maintains the behaviour isn’t the greeting. It’s the ability to prepare.
Not being caught off guard.
Not being blindsided.
Being able to assess, plan, act, or communicate.
It’s a subtle form of agency. And it reminds me that what’s reinforcing isn’t always what we think it is. It’s not always food or toys or smiles. Sometimes it’s the ability to manage the world, even just a little!

References (for the behaviour nerds!):

Balleine, B. W., & Dickinson, A. (1998). Goal-directed instrumental action: contingency and incentive learning and their cortical substrates. Neuropharmacology, 37(4–5), 407–419. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0028-3908(98)00033-1

Boissy, A., & Erhard, H. W. (2014). How studying interactions between animal emotions, cognition, and personality can contribute to improve farm animal welfare. In Genetics and the behavior of domestic animals (pp. 95-129). Academic Press. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-323-85752-9.00014-7

Domjan, M. (2018). The Principles of Learning and Behavior (8th Ed.). Cengage.
Ferster, C. B., & Skinner, B. F. (1957). Schedules of Reinforcement. Appleton-Century-Crofts.

Fureix, C., & Meagher, R. K. (2015). What can inactivity tell us about affective states in non-human animals? Animal Welfare, 24(2), 147–161. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.08.036

Leotti, L. A., Iyengar, S. S., & Ochsner, K. N. (2010). Born to choose: The origins and value of the need for control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 14(10), 457–463. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tics.2010.08.001

Mineka, S., & Hendersen, R. (1985). Controllability and predictability in acquired motivation. Annual Review of Psychology, 36, 495–529. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.ps.36.020185.002431

Nevin, J. A., McLean, A. P., & Grace, R. C. (2001). Resistance to extinction: Contingency termination and generalization decrement. Animal Learning & Behavior, 29(2), 176-191. https://doi.org/10.3758/BF03192826

Seligman, M. E., & Maier, S. F. (1967). Failure to escape traumatic shock. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 74(1), 1–9. https://doi.org/10.1037/h0024514

Wemelsfelder, F. (2007). How animals communicate quality of life: The qualitative assessment of behaviour. Animal Welfare, 16(S1), 25–31. doi:10.1017/S0962728600031699

Wiley, R. H. (2006). Signal detection and animal communication. Advances in the Study of Behavior, 36, 217-247. https://doi.org/10.1016/S0065-3454(06)36005-6

My beautiful old lady ❤️ Billie Jean ❤️
03/08/2025

My beautiful old lady ❤️ Billie Jean ❤️

31/07/2025

I’m trying to trance here 😄

30/07/2025

Herbaceous Health Hoaxes: Why Are We So Afraid of Feeding Dogs Real Food?

The internet is flooded with misleading lists of “toxic” foods for dogs. Online lists warning about “toxic” foods for pets are often bloated with confusion, fear, and half-truths.

But according to FEDIAF (the European Pet Food Industry Federation), only three foods and one supplement are truly toxic to dogs and cats:
• Grapes🍇 (and raisins)
• Chocolate🍫 (cocoa)
• Onions🧅 and their relatives (including chives and high-dose garlic extract—though fresh garlic is fine in moderation)

Compare this short list to the extensive “no-no” food lists found on websites like the ASPCA and AKC, and your head will spin.

Most of those longer lists mix up:
• Truly toxic foods
• Foods that should be avoided for pets with specific health conditions (like pancreatitis)
• Foods that can pose a choking hazard (like pits or whole plants)

For example, eggs🥚, seeds, and nuts🥜 are often wrongly labeled as toxic simply because they’re higher in fat. But these are healthy, nutrient-dense options for most dogs.

Even foods like almonds, peaches, tomatoes, and cherries are only risky if pits or stems are not removed.

Sadly, four truly toxic items have been lumped together with dozens of misunderstood or situationally inappropriate foods, fueling unnecessary fear

The takeaway?
• Avoid grapes, raisins, chocolate, onions, and garlic supplements.

• Use common sense and real science for everything else.
Nutrition doesn't have to be scary—just informed. European common sense for the win.

Here are some common canine food myths we can finally put to rest:

➤ “Avocados🥑 are toxic.” — FALSE.
The myth that avocados are toxic to pets is based on a controversial report filed in 1994 in Nairobi, Kenya, of two malnourished South African dogs who ate the stems and leaves of avocados (Onderstepoort J Vet Res, 1994, 61:107). In fact, a later study showed dogs fed an extract of Avocado flesh, skin and pit🥑 for 6 months was well tolerated with no health or safety concerns.

This report highlights why to keep your pet away from plant stems and leaves, not avocado flesh. (Your pet should also not eat stems and leaves from many plants, including tomato plants and walnut trees.) Avocado pits and skins are also a choking hazard, so don’t feed these parts.

➤ “Never feed dogs mushrooms🍄‍🟫.” — FALSE.
Mushrooms that are safe for people are safe for dogs. (Some mushrooms like wild outdoor mushrooms can be toxic) Likewise, highly medicinal mushrooms for humans are also medicinal for dogs—and the same goes for toxicity. Cooking mushrooms improves digestibility and enhances their benefits by inactivating compounds like agaritine, a naturally occurring mycotoxin found in portobellos.

➤ “Rosemary causes seizures.” — SOMEONE’S CONFUSED.
This myth likely comes from the essential oils of rosemary and eucalyptus, which contain concentrated camphor. In large amounts, this compound can trigger seizures in epileptic animals. But a pinch of fresh rosemary or a dash of dried in your healthy dog’s food is completely safe—and even beneficial.

➤ “Walnuts are toxic.” — PSEUDOSCIENCE.
Raw, unsalted English walnuts (along with almonds and Brazil nuts) can pose a choking risk, so chop them into small pieces first.

Only macadamia nuts are known to cause toxicity (mainly nausea). Peanuts may contain trace mycotoxins, but they are not inherently toxic to dogs.

If you have a black walnut tree, keep your dog away from the bark and outer husk, which can sometimes grow mold that causes vomiting or neurological symptoms.

➤ “Garlic🧄 is toxic.” - FALSE.

📝 Note about garlic🧄:
Garlic often gets a bad reputation because it belongs to the onion family. However, onions contain about 15x more thiosulfate, the compound linked to Heinz body anemia in dogs.

A 2004 study found no anemia in dogs even with high doses of garlic—while noting significant cardiovascular benefits from allicin, garlic’s active compound. That’s why many commercial pet foods safely include garlic, and most vets have no issue with it in appropriate amounts.

More foods that are not toxic for your pet:

• Peaches🍑, cherries🍒, apricots, and other pitted fruits: These fruits are perfectly safe as long as the pits and stems are removed.

• Pork🐖: Some say pork is too fatty for pets, but it actually contains about one-third the fat of beef. Pork is a great source of protein and amino acids and may be ideal for pets with chicken or beef allergies. If feeding raw, the CDC recommends freezing pork for 20 days at 5°F (-15°C) to kill any trichinella parasites. Cooking pork to 145°F (63°C) also makes it safe.

• Salmon🐟: Raw salmon from the Pacific Northwest can, in rare cases, carry a parasite that causes “salmon poisoning” in dogs. The risk is easily avoided by freezing (-20°C for 24 hours) or lightly cooking the salmon.

Source: The Forever Dog & The Forever Dog LIFE
More study links in comments

Billie Jean
30/07/2025

Billie Jean

Love ❤️❤️❤️
29/07/2025

Love ❤️❤️❤️

28/07/2025

Escalation and issues can start subtly.
Play can change in an instant.
Are you witnessing play or discomfort, from either dog.
We have a responsibility to advocate for both dogs involved.
Not just our own.
Step in if there are those little emotional shifts.

That sudden stiffening.

Those vocalisations which now have an "edge" to them.

The fast play that suddenly looks a bit frantic now.

Is the holding down of one of the dogs becoming more intense..... is there more control trying to be exerted?

Play is only play if both dogs are enjoying it.

Love this picture 🥰
28/07/2025

Love this picture 🥰

Address

Patricia
Adelaide, SA

Opening Hours

Monday 8am - 2pm
Tuesday 8am - 2pm
Wednesday 9am - 2pm
Thursday 8am - 2pm
Friday 9am - 5pm
Sunday 9am - 5pm

Telephone

+61413000890

Website

Alerts

Be the first to know and let us send you an email when Alsueno Bull Terriers and Training posts news and promotions. Your email address will not be used for any other purpose, and you can unsubscribe at any time.

Contact The Business

Send a message to Alsueno Bull Terriers and Training:

Share

Category